CHAPTER I
The Historical
Development of Hypnotherapy
It is the purpose of this opening chapter to
trace briefly the historic roots of hypnotherapy, in order to show first the
persistence of therapeutic applications of hypnosis, and secondly, the manifold
attempts to rationalize these applications in some systematic way which would link
the therapy with the presumed etiology of the illness. The frames of reference
into which the phenomenon of hypnosis has been placed range from the strictly
religious systems of the Egyptians, the Persians, and the Hindus, to the most
sophisticated theories of present-day psychology.
The theoretical constructs developed to explain
the phenomena and to guide the therapeutic applications of hypnosis have
evolved along a fairly consistent line; they have moved from a belief in the
exclusive importance of physical or physiological processes to a recognition of
the significance of psychological factors, and recently include some tentative
efforts to achieve, at least in theory, a synthesis of the two. Except for
those clinicians who have used hypnosis as a tool without reference to any
psychopathology, the specific therapeutic applications have been clear
reflections of the theoretical system held to by the therapist.
To date, no satisfactory formulation has been
reached. It is our belief that the curious disparity which exists between the
rich empirical observations of hypnotherapy, and the primitive state of our
understanding of them, is intelligibile only as a reflection inherent in the
difficulties in the exploration of hypnosis, and of the youthfulness of the
science of psychology. We feel, therefore, that the theoretical
systematizations that have been attempted thus far are perforce inadequate, but
that at present we are in a somewhat better position to evaluate these attempts
in the light of the investigations of Freud and his followers.
It is well known that the first effort to bring
hynotherapy within the realm of science was made by Anton Mesmer during the
18th Century. In line with the dominant scientific interest of his day, he
attempted to erect a psychopathology based on disturbances of the
"universal fluids" in which man and all the planets are immersed, his
cures being mediated through the power of "magnets." Apparently he
believed that the iron rods to which his patients held firmly during the seances
were the immediate source of the therapeutic power, although he did regard
"magnetism" as a human emanation. The next important historical step
occurred when the "mesmerists" split among themselves, one group
seeking a scientific or rational basis for mesmeric phenomena, the other
devoting themselves to developing "mesmerism" as an aspect of
clairvoyance. After Mesmer's death, only a few scattered individuals, mainly in
Germany and in England, continued these investigations.
The major turning point in the history of
hypnotherapy occurred during the fourth decade of the 19th Century, when James
Braid (2), an English doctor and himself at first an arch-skeptic, devoted
himself to a re-examination of the so-called "mesmeric phenomena." At
first, Braid attempted to consider these as strictly neurophysiological
occurrences, and accordingly his therapeutic efforts were regarded by him as
attempts to "excite" or "depress" the "nervous
energy" of the patient. He believed at first that the secret of
"mesmerism" was to be discovered by researches in physiological
processes. Later he abandoned this position, and stated that the important
point was the subjective or psychological character of these phenomena.(*)
(*) It has frequently been
claimed by historians (3) (21) (37) that investigators like Bertrand, the Abbe
Faria, and others had anticipated Braid, but inasmuch as it was Braid who
elaborated this psychological view and re-christened the technique
"hypnosis," he is usually given the credit.
In his discussion of the evolution of
psychotherapy, Janet (20) points out the efforts of all psychotherapists to
integrate their findings within some larger conceptual system. Speaking of the
eternal efforts of man to effect intelligible influences on both his inner and
outer worlds, he says: "The forces that he summons to help him were first
the gods, then by a natural evolution they became natural forces, but very
mysterious forces, acting according to unknown laws analoguos, in short, to
divine caprice." Thus, the importance of Braid's investigations seems to
be his effort to bring the phenomena of hypnotherapy within the frame first of
neurophysiology and later of an ill-defined psychology, even as Mesmer had
leaned on astronomy in his attempt to go beyond the mere observation of
phenomena.
It appears, however, that Braid's work was only
a first and tentative step in the direction of wresting the facts of hypnosis
from both a vague astronomy and a prescientific and speculative
neurophysiology. The bitter and extended struggle between the
"physicalists" of Paris, led by Charcot (5), and the
"suggestionists" of Nancy, under Liebeault (23) and Bernheim (1), had
yet to take place before the issue could become clear-cut. Although the work of
Charcot was more experimental than therapeutic, and his observations were
limited to 12 hysterics at the Salpetriere Clinic (3), his prestige as a
neurologist and his emphasis on the tangible made him a formidable opponent to
those who championed the elusive "psychological factor" in hypnosis.
Bramwell (3) and Janet (20) have shown that the Nancy-Paris controversy was
essentially a sophisticated revival of the historic struggle between Braid and
the early mesmerists. Logically this period of debate was unnecessary, but
historically it was inevitable. Bramwell says: "All the old errors, the
result of ignoring mental influences, are once more revived. Medicines are
again alleged to exercise
an influence from within sealed tubes. The
physical and mental conditions of one subject are stated to be transferable to
another, or even to an inanimate object. It is useless to enter into any
arguments to refute these statements; for this would be needlessly repeating
the work of Braid. Indeed, in many instances, their absurdity renders argument
unnecessary : for example when a sealed tube containing laurel-flower water was
brought near a Jewish prostitute, she adored the Virgin Mary! From this it
might be inferred that different religious beliefs were represented by
different nerve centers, and that these could be called into action by
appropriate physical stimuli." (Page 300)
In the context of this dispute during the
classical period of hypnosis in the '90's, Bernheim's concept of suggestion was
a more meaningful and less pallid notion than its current use implies. He used
the term "suggestion" to stress that the essential mechanisms at work
in hypnosis are psychological rather than physical. This was of crucial
importance while the battle raged; but since the victory of the
"suggestionists," the concept of suggestion has lost its impact as a
differentiating idea and has become a shallow cover-all behind which our
ignorance of the specific psychological mechanisms hides. Even the layman no
longer supposes that hypnosis is a function of medicines in sealed tubes; he
"knows" it is "merely a matter of suggestion." This was the
temporary blind alley into which the triumph of the psychological approach led.
It had been proved that the stubborn facts of hypnosis could not be fitted into
a physiological frame of reference, but there was as yet not even a semblance
of an organized set of psychological concepts into which to integrate these
facts. Accordingly, the wild enthusiasm accorded hypnosis during the last two
decades of the 19th Century waned. Janet (21) says in this connection:
"What was it that the school of Nancy put in place of this fine dream? A
few vague assertions on suggestibility and credulity that could not be
discussed or understood without going into the new studies of psychology. But
psychology, considered a confused mixture of literature and ethics, had no
standing in the school of medicine, and indeed it is a question whether
psychology as it was at that period deserved to be more studied by physicians.
It may be added that the enthusiastic exaggerations of the hypnotists had
brought about the random application of hypnotism in all sorts of disease,
without any indication of its fitness, and that the usual results were
meaningless or absurd."
The subsequent history of hypnotherapy is
largely a record of the attempts of various individuals to use hypnosis in
psychotherapy and to formulate a systematic psychopathology within which to
operate. Although it might be said that from the beginning the attempt to fit hypnosis into a larger framework
inevitably involved some assumptions regarding the etiology of mental illness,
the more advanced applications
of hypnosis in psychotherapy became much more tightly bound to
implicit
or stated systems of psychopathology. Most practitioners, however, con
tinued to follow Bernheim in commanding
symptom-disappearance "in a clear loud voice (1)." Others drew back
in fear from the "irrationalities" of hypnosis, preferring to deal
with more readily understandable phenomena and to appeal to the "common
sense" of the patient; for the most part, these men rejected hypnosis in
their psychotherapeutic practice, and opposed it in their theories. This
movement, brought to its most articulate expression by Du Bois (8) and,
according to Hart (17), somewhat less puritanically by Dejerine (6), was
perhaps the last stand of those psychotherapists who preferred to retain the
illusion of "man as a rational animal." Hart has pointed out,
however, that even these active opponents of hypnosis and
"suggestion" were forced to utilize the patient's emotions as levers
in their therapy, whether or not they admitted or even recognized this fact.
Some of the efforts to establish a new rationale
for hypnotherapy overlapped in time with the work of Bernheim and Charcot; but
inasmuch as the general direction of these attempts was significantly different
from the older views, we shall discuss them separately. These were all efforts
to meet the gap left by the abdication of the "physicalists," by
developing a set of psychological concepts to explain both the origins of
mental illnesses and the role of hypnosis in their treatment. We shall reserve
extended discussion of current theories for a later chapter, and mention them
only briefly here in order to complete the chronological development.
It is of considerable interest that all these
transitional psycho-pathologies took as their point of departure the fact first
elaborated in the work on hypnosis, that mental activity may take place in
human beings without their being directly aware of it. The development and
elucidation of this fundamental discovery leads to the next important series of
struggles in the history of psychotherapy in general and hypnotherapy in
particular.
Pierre Janet was one of the first who tried to
establish links between etiology and therapy (21). Proceeding from his clinical
observation and the results of experimental hypnosis, he developed his
well-known theory of "dissociation." At first he drew simply on the
facts of hypnotic hypermnesia, and on his observations of clinical syndromes in
which there appeared to be a "split" or a "disaggregation of
consciousness." Later (20) he accounted for hypnotic phenomena and for
pathological states (fugues, amnesias, hysterical signs) by ascribing both to a
weakening of the "mental synthesis" of the individual. He believed
that mental illness was always such a lack of synthesis, and that this
represented a more or less reversible state of emotional
"bankruptcy." According to him, this weakness made possible the
splitting off of certain mental contents fromthe mainstream of personal
consciousness. He tried to give
this theory
a greater specifity by suggesting that once the weakness
existed, those mental contents charged with strong emotion"
would be
the more likely to dissociate themselves (20). Janet's
complete psycho
pathology was an elaborate analogy between the
human psyche and a great budgetary system. Thus, the aim of all psychotherapy
was to restore solvency where bankruptcy had led to dissociation or to
excessively costly psychic expenditure. If the difficulty lay in the fact that
the patient's "debits" were greater than his "credits," it
followed that attempts should be made either to institute economies or to
provide new forms of "psychic income." Hypnosis could be applied in
either way. The recovery of a dissociated traumatic memory was "an economy
through liquidation," whereas direct suggestion was an attempt to
strengthen the resources of psychically-impoverished patients; they were
particularly receptive to such "contributions" while in hypnosis,
itself a state of weakened synthesis or "dissociation." Janet was an
acute observer and an earnest thinker, but his psychopathology still lacked the
nuclear elements of a dynamic psychology; his application of hypnotherapy was
accordingly limited. Zilboorg (37) says of him: "Janet actually used the
word 'unconscious,' seemingly ascribing to it the value of a truly dynamic
psychological factor, but he stood before his own excellent description and
failed to draw the consequences."
(Page 376)
The "consequences" for a dynamic
psychopathology were drawn by Sigmund Freud as a direct result of his
applications of hypnotherapy in collaboration with Joseph Breuer (4). The
latter had discovered the fact that in hypnosis a significant memory might be
re-lived, and that this "discharge" of emotion had a therapeutic
value. His theoretical systematization of these observations was very close to
Janet's, although he used a different vocabulary. Freud had watched Charcot,
Liebeault, and Bernheim at work and had been impressed. He tried to interest
Charcot in Breuer's method of permitting the patient to talk freely and to
express deep feeling, but Charcot was indifferent. Janet, independent of
Breuer, had made the same innovation with his hypnotized patients (19) but had stopped short of the final
implications of his own work.
The critical last step was taken by Freud alone
(15). Unlike Janet, he was not content with the belief that ideas are
"split off" sheerly as a result of an inherent weakness of the
person, or a quantitative deficit of emotional energy. He felt such a split
must take place in a less arbitrary, more lawful way. Such a dissociation was
for him not a mere lack of cohesion or a mechanical psychic crumbling, but a
systematic necessity for the person: the dissociated set of impulses must be
qualitatively different from the mainstream of personal consciousness, and must
have been purposively "pushed" from awareness in the patient's effort
to protect himself from their onslaught. Freud devoted himsell to the elaboration and development
of this revolutionary finding, and abandoned the exploration of hypnotherapy within this framework. He
gradually built
the therapeutic techniques of psychoanalysis
and, from his observations, the first dynamic psychopathology.
Now, for the first time, there existed the
possibility of viewing hypnosis and hypnotherapy as rational phenomena within
the conceptual framework of a scientific psychology; but the accidents of
history precluded this. The founder of the new set of concepts had stated that
"psychoanalysis . . . only began with my rejection of the hypnotic
technique (11)." Thus, for a long time after the publication of Studies
in Hysteria (4) Freud and his followers avoided the therapeutic application
of hypnosis, and only a few isolated attempts were made to suggest a
psychoanalytic theory of hypnosis. Freud made one such attempt in his effort to
integrate the phenomena of hypnosis with his conception of the role of the
group-leader and with the fact of "falling in love (14)." Ferenczi
(9), Schilder (32) and others formulated provocative but speculative theories
based on the idea that hypnosis is essentially "a resuscitation of
infantile erotic masochistic adjustments." Both Ferenczi and Schilder tried
to accumulate clinical data to substantiate their theories; although they
present some interesting evidence it seems clear that a good deal more will be
necessary before a well-founded theory is possible.
Anna Freud (10), following the leads suggested
at the outset by her father (4), has tried to show that hypnosis in therapy
seeks to eliminate the ego with all of its defense mechanisms, thus concealing
the resistance of the patient. Although this view has not been substantiated by
clinical observation, it is at least a hypothesis conceived in the theoretical
framework of a scientific psychology and amenable to empirical check.
It has been unfortunate for the development of
hypnotherapy that it was a historical necessity for Freud to reject hypnosis.
This left both its practice and theory to those who were desperately fighting
the insights of psychoanalysis. The psychological theories of hypnosis advanced
in the first decades of this century were, with but a few notable exceptions
(26), either variations on Janet's concept of "dissociation" or
hybrid compromises between this view and the Freudian theories. There were also
offered elaborate but highly speculative neurophysiological theories (26) (29)
which led to no new advance. The practice of hypnotherapy therefore simply
followed one of two old paths: the removal of symptoms by direct suggestion, or
the attempt to remove symptoms by hypnotically inducing a re-living of one or
more specific traumatic episodes.
Typical for this period are the researches of Morton Prince (29)
(30), William Mc Dougall
(24) ( 26), and Boris
Sidis (33) (350 (36). Prince, by far the most
conservative of the three,
devoted more ofhis energy to hypnotic experiment and
exploration than to therapy.
He believed like the others in the existence of
menial processes inaccessible to direct awareness, but he insisted on their
"logical, orderly" character (31). He said: "Now a word as to my
conception of the unconscious. I conceive the unconscious not as a wild,
unbridled, conscienceless subconscious mind, as do some Freudians, ready to
take advantage of an unguarded moment to strike down, to drown, to kill, after
the manner of an evil genii, but as a great mental mechanism which takes part
in an orderly, logical way in all the processes of daily life but which under
certain conditions involving particularly the emotion-instincts, becomes
disordered or perverted. We think that in everyday life we consciously do the
whole of our thinking; but I am inclined to think that our unconscious self
does most of our thinking, and that we simply select from the ideas furnished
by the unconscious those which we believe are best adapted to the situation;
that our problems are much more solved by the unconscious than by the
conscious. I regard the normal unconscious as a logical, orderly mind, playing
an important part in everyday life. Therefore, as I see it, this conception of
unconscious ideas is fundamental, and not the libido, although the instincts
play a tremendous role in carrying, by the force of their conative impulses,
ideas to fruition and setting up conflicts between opposing ideas with
resulting disharmony."
This notion of a dissociated consciousness, in
essence similar to that of consciousness as we ordinarily think of it, actually
goes back to the early discoveries of (so-called) unconscious processes by
workers in hypnosis (16) who felt they had proved their existence by showing
that an arithmetical problem could be solved without the subject having any
awareness of this process. The belief in the essentially rational nature of the
unconscious necessarily leads to an over-rationalistic application of hypnosis
in therapy, and completely neglects the role of instinctual strivings, as well
as the nature of symptom-development and defense mechanisms. Prince's
theoretical position was very close to that of Janet; McDougall, on the other
hand, while rejecting violently what he termed the "pan-sexuality" of
the Freudians, represented an important advance in that his concept of the
unconscious included a recognition of the existence of purposive strivings. He
developed an independent psychology, firmly rooted in a belief in the
importance of instinctual drives; his therapeutic applications of hypnosis were
thus less academic than those of Prince. Sidis, too, although holding to a
belief in "disaggregations of consciousness," was closer to a dynamic
position. None the less, he launched bitter and scathing denunciations of
psychoanalysis, and built
his own psychology and psychopathology largely
on the "instinct of
fear(36)." His most important contribution
perhaps lies in his distinction between therapies employing "direct"
and "indirect" suggestion (34). He regarded as "indirect
suggestion" the use of trickery and deception in therapy (the use of
placebos, faradic brushes, etc.). In contrast to this he described the
"direct suggestion" of hypnosis, which may be successful in the
absence of faith or of credulity. Thus he attacks the nebulous concept of
"suggestion," and explores the possibility of giving it some specificity.
The implications of this view for the practical applications of hypnotherapy
are self-evident.
Since the work of these men during the first
decades of this century, little systematic effort has been made to understand
hypnosis as a rational phenomenon or to develop it as a tool of psychotherapy
within the frame of our modern psychology. Many investigators have tried to do
both when dealing with circumscribed experimental or clinical problems; but
aside from the work of Schilder and Kauders (32), summarized by them in 1927,
there has been no comprehensive statement of the role of hypnosis in modern
psychotherapy.
We have seen that hypnotherapy has undergone a
historical evolution which has reflected the history of the science of
psychology and the development of psychopathology, and that the therapeutic
applications have been a direct expression of these. The attempt to understand
hypnosis in terms of physical forces or neurophysiological processes failed;
this position gave way to an approach which has emphasized psychological
factors which we are only now beginning to understand. A synthesis of these
positions has seemed impossible. However, the current researches on mechanical
adjuvants to hypnotherapy, and on the relationship between drugs and hypnotic
states, suggests that an integration may finally be achieved.
We have omitted in this chapter many of the
great names in the history of hypnotherapy, because our aim has been not to
record the clinical achievements of hypnotherapy, but the significant
theoretical developments which altered clinical practice.
|